Patent No. 5406956 Method and apparatus for truth detection
Patent No. 5406956
Method and apparatus for truth detection (Farwell, Apr 18, 1995)
Abstract
A method of detecting information stored in the brain of a subject includes presenting to the subject in oddball series Probe, Target, and Irrelevant stimuli. The Probe stimuli are relevant to a situation under investigation; the Irrelevant stimuli are not; and the Target stimuli are identified to the subject as being noteworthy, and in response to which the subject is instructed to perform a task. The Target stimuli like the Probe stimuli are relevant to the situation under investigation. The method also includes detecting electrical brain responses for each of the stimuli; analyzing the responses for uncovering an event related brain potential; and comparing the Probe responses with the Target responses to determine whether the subject recognizes the Probes, and comparing the Probe responses with the Irrelevant responses to determine whether the subject does not recognize the Probes. Three exemplary headbands are disclosed for positioning electrodes at preferred locations on the subject's scalp for obtaining electrical responses therefrom.
Notes:
INTRODUCTION
Conventional Psychophysiological Detection
of Concealed Information
Attempts to discover concealed information stored in the brain of another human
being are as old as human history. The purpose of this report is to propose
a new paradigm for the psychophysiological detection of concealed information,
to test the new paradigm, to contrast it with the conventional paradigm, and
to examine its theoretical and practical implications.
When an individual has a particular significant experience, information regarding
that experience is stored in the brain. The task at hand for another person
lacking knowledge of the original event who wishes to discover the facts regarding
the event is to detect this concealed information stored in the brain.
For millennia, the method for achieving this end has been interrogation. Interrogation
consists of asking the person questions regarding the information of interest,
and attempting to motivate him/her to reveal the information (Aubry & Caputo,
1980). The Farwell Truth Detector introduced here detects the concealed information
in a different manner: by measuring electrical brain activity that results,
in certain circumstances, from the presence of the concealed information.
In conventional detection of concealed information, when an individual is questioned
regarding information that he or she may be motivated to conceal, some means
of assessing the credibility of the statements made must be practiced in order
for the questioning to be productive (Richardson, 1963). Credibility assessment
can be achieved in two ways: 1) through observation of overt behavior, and 2)
through observation of non-overt behavior through psychophysiological measurement.
Although the detection of concealed information and credibility assessment through
psychophysiological means have been practiced successfully for thousands of
years, the utilization of scientific instrumentation in this pursuit has been
practiced for only about a century.
This practice is generally known as lie detection, detection of deception, or
polygraphy. What is actually detected, however, is neither lies nor deception;
and there are many other uses of the polygraph. Thus, the term "interrogative
polygraphy" is perhaps a more suitable term. I shall use the term "psychophysiological
detection of concealed information" as inclusive of both interrogative polygraphy
and alternative methods of detection involving the observation of psychophysiological
responses without instrumentation.
Prior to the research reported here, interrogative polygraphy and all other
recorded attempts to utilize psychophysiological measures in the service of
detecting concealed information were based on an emotional/physiological arousal
paradigm (Munsterberg, 1908; Reid & Inbau, 1977; Matte, 1980; Ansley, 1975).
It is well known that when faced with a potentially threatening situation, the
body prepares itself to become vigorously, physically active in order to deal
with that situation. This preparation takes place when strong emotions are aroused,
even if an actual physical response of fighting or fleeing is known to be out
of the question. Psychophysiological methods of detecting concealed information
have been founded on eliciting such emotions (Ferguson & Miller, 1973; Gugas,
1979).
The fundamental procedure that has been developed based on this phenomenon combines
interrogation and psychophysiological credibility assessment (Inbau & Reid,
1953; 1962; Aubry & Caputo, 1980). There are three aspects to this procedure:
1) The subject is questioned regarding the event in question, and his/her answers
are noted.
2) The subject is confronted with questions or statements regarding the event
in question, in a context that is potentially threatening or fear inducing.
According to the theory, a subject who has actual information that he has not
accurately revealed (a "guilty" subject) will experience greater negative emotions
than a subject who is not concealing information (an "innocent" subject).
3) According to the theory, this negative emotion will result in a physiological
activation preparatory for strenuous physical activity that can be measured
through various channels--primarily perspiration on the palms, changes in cardiovascular
activity, and changes in breathing patterns (Reid & Inbau, 1977; Matte,
1980; Ferguson & Miller, 1974). (Several other responses, including salivation,
peripheral blood flow, and pupillary responses, have also been measured.)
What is actually measured is the physiological response. According to the theory
of conventional interrogative polygraphy, a large physiological response indicates
a large emotional response, which in turn indicates guilt or deception (Ferguson
& Miller, 1973; Matte, 1980). As will be described in detail in subsequent
sections, this simple theory has remained essentially unchanged for two thousand
years (Ferguson & Miller, 1974). This is not to say that there has been
no progress in interrogative polygraphy during that time. The progress that
has taken place--and, particularly during the last quarter century, this has
been extensive--has been of an empirical nature. As a recent report prepared
under the auspices of the Department of Defense (Department of Defense, 1984)
concluded, "The polygraph field is one of those rare situations where the practice
has outpaced the research."
Polygraphers have had extensive opportunities to develop a database consisting
of physiological responses on the one hand and confessions or other determinations
of guilt or innocence on the other. An extensive and systematic body of knowledge
has developed regarding the specific patterns of responses that correspond with
an ultimate determination that the subject was guilty or innocent (Munsterberg,
1908; Marston, 1917; Larson, 1922, 1932, 1969; Keeler, 1930; Reid & Inbau,
1977; Raskin, 1989). In parallel with this, much has been learned about criminal
psychology and the interrogative techniques that are most effective in eliciting
revealing responses, both overt and psychophysiological (Munsterberg, 1908;
Inbau & Reid, 1962; Gugas, 1979; Aubry & Caputo, 1980).
A simple theory provided the original impetus to make physiological measurements.
For the purpose of practical results in investigations, the theoretical foundations,
theoretical implications, and intervening variables are not of primary importance
(Department of Defense, 1984). What has been systematically and extensively
investigated is 1) what physiological patterns can be taken as reliable indicators
of guilt, and 2) what techniques of interrogation are most effective in making
use of these responses (Reid & Inbau, 1977; Aubry & Caputo, 1980).
In order to detect information that is stored in the brain, conventional interrogative
polygraphy resorts to interrogations to elicit true or false statements, and
psychophysiological credibility assessment or lie detection to determine whether
the subject can be believed or not. The lie detection procedure involves presenting
a stimulus that is relevant to the concealed information in a potentially threatening
or upsetting situation and measuring physiological responses--on the theory
that a guilty individual will emit a greater emotional response to the relevant
stimuli, and this greater emotional response will be reflected in a greater
physiological response (Munsterberg, 1908; Marston, 1917; Selling, 1938; Ansley,
1975; Matte, 1980; Gale, 1988, Raskin, 1989).
The theoretical basis, then, of conventional psychophysiological detection of
concealed information is a general arousal theory. Since the origin of interrogative
polygraphy, psychophysiological research has gone beyond a general theory of
arousal to explore a variety of more specific psychophysiological responses
related to emotions (Lacey, 1967; Obrist, Webb, Sutterer, & Howard, 1970;
Schwartz, Weinberger, & Singer, 1981; Mathews, May, Mogg, & Eysenck,
1990; Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, & Friesen, 1990; Ekman, Davidson,
& Friesen, 1990; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990; Eysenck, Mogg, May,
Richards, & Mathews, 1991). This further development, however, has not fundamentally
changed the theory of interrogative polygraphy. Progress in conventional interrogative
polygraphy has consisted primarily of an empirical investigation of what psychophysiological
responses are indicative of guilt or deception. A variety of different patterns
have been shown to be reliable predictors in this regard (Reid & Inbau,
1977; Matte, 1980; Ansley, 1975).
Two closely related areas where considerable theoretical, as well as practical,
advances have taken place in recent years are criminal assessment through observation
of overt behavior, and criminal psychology and interrogation (Richardson, 1963;
Inbau & Reid, 1962; Gugas, 1979; Aubry & Caputo, 1980). These are beyond
the scope of this paper.
Remaining within the realm of general arousal theory, two different methods
of interrogative polygraphy employing brain waves could be designed. Alpha waves
have been hypothesized to be an indicator of cortical deactivation and general
relaxation, and both alpha blocking and beta waves have been hypothesized to
be an indicator of cortical activation. Although these hypotheses are not universally
accepted, they have received a considerable measure of empirical support. (See,
for example, Nowlis & Kamiya, 1970; Brown, 1971; Andersen & Andersson,
1968; Lippold & Novotny, 1970; Shaw, Foley, & Blowers, 1970; Plotkin
& Cohen, 1976; Travis, Kondo, & Knott, 1975; Lynch & Paskewitz,
1971; Davidson, 1984a, 1984b; 1987; Kinsbourne & Bemporad, 1984; Leventhal
& Tomarken, 1986; Silberman & Weingartner, 1986; Tucker & Frederick,
1989; Davidson & Tomarken, 1989; Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, &
Friesen, 1990). Based on this, one could reasonably advance the hypothesis that
the presentation of a crime-relevant stimulus would elicit greater alpha blocking
and/or greater beta activity in a guilty person than in an innocent person.
Another theory holds that voltage potential at the scalp is an indication of
the level of activation of the brain. There is strong evidence that this is
the case in certain limited circumstances, particularly situations involving
motor potentials (Kornhuber & Deeke, 1965; Kutas & Donchin, 1980). Some
have theorized that slow event-related brain potentials are the result of modulations
in the level of widespread cortical activation: negative potentials indicate
activation; apparently positive potentials can be explained as transient deactivation
(Desmedt, 1980; Verleger, 1988). If this theory is correct, then one could on
this basis predict increased cortical negativity in a guilty subject's response
to crime-relevant stimuli, since these stimuli would, as indicated by other
measures, result in a higher level of activation. As will be described in detail
in subsequent sections, the findings predicted in this paper are incompatible
with this theory and this predicted result.
The New Paradigm
This paper proposes a new paradigm regarding concealed information and its relationship
to psychophysiological measurements, and describes a series of experiments implementing
and evaluating this paradigm. The paradigm is distinct from the emotional/physiological
arousal paradigm, based on a general arousal theory, that has formed the basis
of interrogative polygraphy to date. If the results are as predicted, this new
paradigm promises to provide a new means of psychophysiological detection of
concealed information that detects the presence of the information more directly
and reliably than the conventional methods--without recourse to the mandatory
elicitation of negative emotions or dependence on the linkage between negative
emotions and physiological arousal.
The P300 Component
The theory and research presented here are based on the P300 component of the
event-related brain potential, also referred to as P3 and P3b. There is considerable
evidence that P300s are elicited by relatively rare stimuli within a series
of more frequent stimuli, when--and only when--these rare stimuli provide information
needed by a subject for the performance of a task assigned by the experimenter.
For example, if a subject is instructed to count low tones in a series of 20%
low and 80% high tones, the low tones will elicit a P300. If the subject is
asked to ignore the tones and solve a mental problem instead, the same tones
do not elicit a P300 (Duncan-Johnson & Donchin, 1977). If a series of photographs
is presented containing a majority of neutral photographs along with a few photographs
of politicians and a few photographs of movie stars, a subject will emit P300s
in response to one but not the other of these categories, depending on which
category he or she is instructed to count (Towle, Heuer, & Donchin, 1980).
P300's, then, have been shown to be elicited by stimuli that are rare and explicitly
task-relevant.
There are two major classes of theories to explain this phenomenon. One class
of theories, exemplified by the context updating theory (Donchin, 1981; Donchin,
Karis, Bashore, Coles, & Gratton, 1986; Donchin & Coles, 1988a; 1988b),
holds that the P300 is the manifestation of an active information processing
"subroutine" in the brain. According to context updating, when the explicitly
task-relevant stimulus arrives, the subject updates his internal representation
of the environment, and it is this active process that is manifested in a P300
on the scalp.
Another class of theories of the functional significance of P300 holds that
the P300 is the result of a transient deactivation of the cortex (Desmedt, 1980;
Verleger, 1988). The cortex has been activated in anticipation of the arrival
of the task-relevant stimulus. When the stimulus arrives "context closure" occurs,
the subject momentarily relaxes his vigilance, and the cortex is deactivated,
resulting in an apparent positivity. Then the cortex is again activated in anticipation
of the next stimulus, resulting in a return to the previous state of cortical
negativity (that appears to be a return to baseline).
Context updating, then, holds the P300 to be a manifestation of the activation
of a specific, information-processing process. Brain deactivation/closure holds
the P300 to be the result of a transient cortical deactivation. Note that these
two theories are not directly comparable. Context updating specifies the functional
significance of P300 in terms of a specific information-processing function;
it does not specify particular brain structures or physiological brain functions.
Brain deactivation specifies particular electrophysiological events in particular
brain areas--specifically, deactivation of certain cortical areas--and explicitly
denies the existence of any specific, concomitant information-processing process.
These differences will be discussed in detail in subsequent sections.
Psychophysiological Detection of Concealed
Information with the P300
If the context updating theory is correct, it may be possible to design an interrogative
polygraphy technique that takes advantage of the specific nature of the information-processing
process manifested in P300 to draw conclusions regarding the information that
is processed--and therefore possessed--by the subject. This is the perspective
taken in the research described here.
On the other hand, if the cortical activation/deactivation theory is valid,
it may be possible to develop a method of interrogative polygraphy through the
elicitation of increased cortical activation and corresponding negativity by
the presentation of crime-relevant stimuli, or through other interrogative techniques
eliciting cortical activation.
The different predictions made by these two theories, and the practical applications
for psychophysiological detection of concealed information, will be discussed
in detail in subsequent sections.
It is well established that P300's are elicited by stimuli that provide information
necessary for the performance of an explicit task assigned by the experimenter
(Sutton, Braren, Zubin, & John, 1965). In previous research, P300's have
been absent in the absence of relevance to such an explicit task (Sutton, Tueting,
Zubin, & John, 1967; Duncan-Johnson & Donchin, 1977).
The hypothesis advanced here is that stimuli that are not explicitly task-relevant
will nevertheless elicit a P300 if they are particularly significant to the
subject due to his past experience with the subject matter of the stimuli. This
is based on the context updating model. If this model is correct, then when
a stimulus that is significant for the individual arrives., he or she can be
expected to take particular note of it, thus revising his/her internal representation
of the current environment and emitting a P300. If this is shown to be the case,
this will lend support to the notion that the P300 is indeed the manifestation
of a particular, active process. Moreover, it will demonstrate that this is
a process that takes place upon the arrival of relevant information even when
the subject is not instructed or motivated to engage in this process--in fact,
when it is to the subject's disadvantage to do so.
Again, previous research has indicated that stimuli will elicit a P300 only
when they are explicitly task-relevant, and not when they are not explicitly
task-relevant even if they are recognized by the subject (e.g., movie stars
when one is counting politicians; Towle, Heuer, and Donchin, 1980). A hypothesis
advanced here is that explicit task relevance is not a necessary condition for
the elicitation of a P300. What could be called "implicit task relevance"--a
high measure of significance for the subject--is a sufficient condition on the
task relevance dimension.
This paper sets forth a series of experiments to test this hypothesis, and also
to investigate further the nature and prerequisites for implicit task relevance
or significance.
The Research Program
The original research program comprises three experiments. (Experiments 1 and
2 were reported by Farwell & Donchin, 1986, 1991. Experiment 3 was reported
by Farwell, 1992.) In Experiment 1, a particular set of stimuli was made implicitly
relevant to a subject by requiring the subject to learn them through an interactive
computer program and to carry out a mock espionage scenario utilizing the relevant
information. On the following day, subjects were presented with the implicitly
relevant "probe" stimuli interspersed with two other types of stimuli: irrelevant
stimuli and target stimuli. All stimuli were visually presented short phrases.
Subjects were asked to memorize a list of targets, and to press one button in
response to targets and another button in response to all others. Target and
probe stimulus probability were 0.17; irrelevant probability were 0.66. Each
subject was tested with two different sets of stimuli, one containing probes
relevant to an espionage scenario he or she has carried out ("guilty" condition),
and one with probes associated with another scenario unknown to the subject
("innocent" condition).
The innocent condition, then, was simply a standard experiment of the kind that
has been shown to produce P300's. Since the subject cannot distinguish the probes
from the irrelevants, he sees only two kinds of stimuli: rare and task-relevant
targets, and frequent irrelevants. A P300 is expected in response to the targets
and not in response to the irrelevants and the probes (which are not recognized
as such).
The guilty condition provides a test of the implicit task relevance hypothesis
and indirectly of the context updating model on which it is based. If my hypothesis
is correct, the subject will display large P300's to probes as well as to targets.
Experiment 2 sought to delineate further the nature of implicit task relevance.
In Experiment 1, probe stimuli are learned only one day prior to testing, and
are learned in the context of the experiment. In Experiment 2, the probe stimuli
consisted of phrases relevant to an actual crime or socially undesirable act
committed by the subject up to several years before testing. Stimulus sets were
developed through discussion with the subject on the day prior to testing. In
other regards, experimental design and predicted results were similar to Experiment
1.
Even if the results of Experiment 2 are as predicted, it is still possible that
the significance of the probes is a function of the discussions that take place
shortly prior to testing. Experiment 3 was designed to test the hypothesis that
personal relevance or implicit task relevance can be achieved on the basis of
significant life events alone, outside of the experimental setting, and that
it can be retained over a period of years. Experimental procedure was as before,
except that all information regarding the personal life events that give rise
to the probe stimuli will be supplied by a third party who knows the subject
well. Subjects will not know what stimuli will be presented or what events will
be investigated prior to the test session. As before, the predicted result for
a guilty subject is large P300's to targets and probes and not to irrelevants.
If the results of Experiment 3 are as predicted, this will serve to delineate
further the nature and time course of implicit task relevance, and provide further
support for the context updating model of the functional significance of the
P300. This will be discussed in detail in the section on "Hypotheses and Theoretical
Significance of this Research."
The permission of each subject will be obtained for the acquisition of information
from each specific informant. Any information so obtained will remain confidential.
These three experiments, if results are as predicted, can serve as the theoretical
and practical foundation of a new form of psychophysiological detection of concealed
information. All previous methods involve interrogation--asking the subject
about the situation under investigation--and credibility assessment--attempts
to determine if the subject is telling the truth (Reid & Inbau, 1977; Matte,
1980; Department of Defense, 1984; Aubry and Caputo, 1980; Raskin, 1986; Lykken,
1978). Credibility assessment in turn involves measurement of physiological
responses on the theory that large physiological responses indicate a large
emotional response, which in turn indicates that the sought after, crime-relevant
information is indeed stored in the brain (Ferguson & Miller, 1973).
The method proposed here seeks to reveal the crime-relevant information stored
in the brain through measurement of the psychophysiological manifestations of
information-processing brain activity. The Farwell Truth Detector described
here does not depend on the elicitation of certain emotions or the measurement
of the putative physiological correlates of emotions. The system creates a situation
such that the presence of crime relevant information stored in the brain--regardless
of emotions or arousal levels--will result in the implementation of a certain
information-processing brain function that can be detected electrophysiologically.
One advantage of this approach is that both emotions and the accompanying autonomic
nervous system arousal can be multiply determined, whereas the process manifested
in the P300 is a quite specific one. On the emotional level, there may be many
reasons other than guilt or falsehood why a person may be aroused (or fail to
be aroused) by being questioned about a crime. Moreover, the autonomic nervous
system is activated not only by emotions. It is responsible for maintaining
homeostasis in the system as a whole. It continually readjusts the balance between
anabolic and catabolic processes, maintains a suitable temperature in the different
areas of the body, supplies nutrients and oxygen to different organs on demand
and according to a complex system of shifting priorities, and makes a multitude
of adjustments in different bodily systems in response to continually changing
conditions in the internal and external environment. Thus, the changes in autonomic
nervous system activity measured by conventional polygraphy may be the result
of a multitude of psychological and physiological factors, some related to the
subject of the investigation and some unrelated. Moreover, a knowledgeable subject
may deliberately engage in psychological or physical activities that influence
autonomic nervous system activity during an examination, and such countermeasures
may have considerable effectiveness (Honts, Hodes, & Raskin, 1985; Honts,
Raskin, & Kircher, 1987).
By contrast, there is strong evidence that the information-processing activity
of which the P300 is a manifestation is a specific one (Donchin, Karis, Bashore,
Coles, & Gratton, 1986). The brain-wave information detection (BID) system
described here attempts to give the subject a task where that particular information-processing
function comes into play if and only if the relevant information is stored in
the brain. As will be described in detail in subsequent sections, the Farwell
Truth Detector attempts in this way to establish a very specific link between
a psychophysiological measurement and the presence or absence of specific, relevant
information stored in the brain.
EARLY PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL DETECTION
OF CONCEALED INFORMATION
The Veda, the earliest record of human experience, contains the first record
of systematic methods for the psychophysiological detection of concealed information.
The Veda is said to be well over 10,000
years old. However, since it was passed down orally for many generations
by pundits who memorized it syllable for syllable and taught it to the upcoming
generation from an early age, it is difficult to ascertain its age accurately.
It may have been first written down about 900 B.C. Ancient Vedic scientists
used the pulse, measured from the wrist, to diagnose a wide variety of physical,
mental, emotional, and spiritual imbalances, including both guilt over past
deeds and tendencies to violence and other crime. The Mitakshara Shastra of
the Ayur Veda, that section of the Vedic literature concerned with both psychophysiology
and health, offered the following method for detecting individuals who had committed
the particularly heinous crime of poisoning:
A person who gives poison may be recognized. He does not answer questions, or
they are evasive answers; he speaks nonsense, rubs the great toe along the ground,
and shivers; his face is discolored; he rubs the roots of the hair with his
fingers; and he tries by every means to leave the house . . . " (Mitakshara
Shastra. See Wise, 1845, p. 394; see also Trovillo, 1939; Horvath, 1973).
Like the methods employed in modern interrogation, this ancient account calls
for the observation of both psychophysiological and overt behavioral symptoms.
The first practice of psychophysiological detection of concealed information
recorded in detail was by the Greek physician and physiologist Erasistratos
in the third century B.C. (Appian of Alexandria, early second century A.D.,
1962; Plutarch, first century A.D./1952; Valerius Maximus, first century A.D./1888;
see also Mosso, 1896; Trovillo, 1939; Horvath, 1973; Mesulam & Perry, 1972).
Erasistratos used psychophysiological detection of concealed information, without
the benefit of modern psychophysiological recording equipment, to solve a particularly
difficult medical case. His patient was a young man named Antiochus, the son
of Seleucus I of Syria, a former general of Alexander the Great who had recently
married a beautiful younger woman named Stratonice. Antiochus fell in love with
his new stepmother, and realizing the hopelessness of his situation, attempted
to hide his passion. He soon became very ill.
Erasistratos concluded that the young man had no physical ailment, and consequently
his illness must have an emotional origin. He made careful psychophysiological
observations of Antiochus' responses to various stimuli in the environment in
order to determine the cause of the illness. Suspecting that the young man's
difficulties arose from his feelings for another individual, and that he was
doing his best to avoid any overt behavior that would give him a way, Erasistratos
made some of the same measurements that are now incorporated in modern polygraph
testing, along with other related observations. Here, in the words of Plutarch
(first century A.D./1952), is what Erasistratos observed:
He . . . waited continually in his chamber, and when any of the beauties of
the court made their visit to the sick prince, he observed the emotions and
alterations in the countenance of Antiochus, and watched for the changes which
he knew to be indicative of the inward passions and inclinations of the soul.
He took notice that the presence of other women produced no effect upon him;
but when Stratonice came as she often did, alone or with Seleucus, to see him,
he observed in him all of Sappho's famous symptoms,--his voice faltered, his
face flushed up, his eyes glanced stealthily, a sudden sweat broke out on his
skin, the beatings of his heart were irregular and violent, and, unable to support
the excess of his passion, he would sink into a state of faintness, prostration,
and pallor.
The "sudden sweat" described by Erasistratos constitutes the primary symptom
recorded by modern interrogative polygraphy. The "irregular and violent beatings
of the heart"--also a response recorded in modern lie detection--could only
have been observable if Erasistratos had actually taken the pulse at the appropriate
times (a fact that was pointed out by another great early Greek physician and
scientist, Galen of Pergamum, second century A.D.).
Here Erasistratos exhibits a sophisticated theoretical understanding of psychophysiology,
the same theoretical understanding that forms the basis of all modern conventional
interrogative polygraphy prior to the system proposed here (Matte, 1980; Ferguson
& Miller, 1973). Erasistratos must have realized that emotions and bodily
functioning are intimately related. He recognized that a stimulus in the external
world could produce a visceral response due to its particular significance for
an individual. He replicated his findings on the relationship of the stimulus
and the psychophysiological response through repeated observations at different
times. Erasistratos also must have understood that the visceral responses could
reveal emotions--and, by inference, the concealed information that gave rise
to them--even if the subject was determined to reveal neither the emotion nor
the facts giving rise to it through any overt means. Moreover, Erasistratos'
scientific study was a controlled one. He assured himself of the specificity
of the stimulus by comparing the subject's response to the critical stimulus
to his responses to the presence of other individuals of the court.
Galen of Pergamum, often hailed as the father of modern medicine and an admirer
of Erasistratos, further developed the theoretical and practical approach introduced
by his predecessor (Mesulam & Perry, 1972). He applied it to solve a similar
problem, this time with a woman suffering from insomnia. After examining her,
he concluded that it was likely that her problem was due to "some trouble she
was unwilling to confess." (Galen, second century A.D.)
While I was convinced the woman was afflicted by not bodily disease, but rather
that some emotional trouble grieved her, it happened that at the very moment
I was examining her this was confirmed. Someone returning from the theater mentioned
he had seen Pylades dancing. Indeed, at that instant, her expression and the
color of her face were greatly altered. Attentive, my hand laid on the woman's
wrist, I observed her pulse was irregular, suddenly violently agitated, which
points to a troubled mind. The same thing occurs in people engaged in an argument
over a given subject.
The next day, I told one of my following that when I went to visit the woman
he was to arrive a little later and mention that Morpheus was dancing that day.
When this was done the patient's pulse was in no way changed. And likewise,
on the following day, while I was attending her, the name of the third dancer
was mentioned, and in like fashion the pulse was hardly affected at all. I investigated
the matter for a fourth time in the evening. Studying the pulse and seeing that
it was excited and irregular when mention was made that Pylades was dancing,
I concluded that the lady was in love with Pylades and in the days following,
this conclusion was confirmed exactly.
Galen's scientific application of psychophysiology to detect concealed information
goes beyond that of Erasistratos. Rather than passively waiting for the arrival
of the critical stimulus, Galen intentionally manipulated the environment to
elicit the responses in question. He used control stimuli as well as relevant
ones. Moreover, he recognized the concept, fundamental to modern interrogative
polygraphy, of stimulus generalization: the name, rather than the individual,
was enough to elicit the response (Mesulam & Perry, 1972). In a criticism
of physicians ignorant of the psychophysiological connection between mind and
body, Galen (second century A.D.) clearly describes the intimate relationship
that forms the basis of modern conventional interrogative polygraphy.
Why did these things escape the notice of earlier physicians attending the lady
described above?. They are arrived at by ordinary deduction, if the physician
has even a meager knowledge of medicine.
Indeed, I think that it is because these physicians possess no clear conception
of the ways the body tends to be influenced by the state of the mind. Perhaps
it is because they do not even know that the pulse becomes turbulent because
of strife and fears which suddenly disturb the mind.
In the tenth century the great Persian physician Avicenna, in his Canon medicinae,
describes similar success to that of Erasistratos and Galen in diagnosing "love-sickness,"
and systematic identification of its object, in an individual who had been unwilling
to confess his condition. Avicenna also expanded the concept of stimulus generalization
to include not only the name of the relevant person but other related items
such as her place of residence. Avicenna's (also known as Ibn Sina, tenth century
A.D) description of his method is extremely similar to the conventional lie
detection practiced by interrogative polygraphers today (Ibn Sina, 1608; Mesulam
& Perry, 1972). In the following passage, Avicenna (Ibn Sina, 1608) describes
the detection of a love-sick individual.
It is possible in this way to ascertain whom he loves, even when he will not
reveal it himself . . . The nature of the cure is this: let several names be
pronounced, repeating them many times, and place your finger on the patient's
pulse. When it varies by a large fluctuation and then returns to normal, and
this is repeated thereafter, and is put to the test many times, then the name
of the one he loves will be known.
Again, similarly, make mention of her looks and habits and that in which she
excels, her family, where she lives, so that any one of these things may be
associated with the name of his loved one. Observe his pulse in such a way that
when it fluctuates at the mention of one of these details the particular characteristics
of his loved one may then be associated with a name and with an outstanding
feature, by all of which she is to be recognized.
It is clear that the theoretical foundations of conventional interrogative polygraphy,
and indeed a recognition of many of the critical psychophysiological responses
to be measured, are founded on a two-thousand-year-old tradition. Modern conventional
interrogative polygraphy has built upon this foundation with sophisticated instrumentation,
and has developed a rich empirical data base.
THE DEVELOPMENT OF MODERN CONVENTIONAL
INTERROGATIVE POLYGRAPHY
Interrogative polygraphy, the use of scientific instrumentation in psychophysiological
detection of concealed information, began with Lombroso in the late nineteenth
century (Lombroso, 1890, 1887, 1911, 1912; Ferraro, 1911). Lombroso made continuous
recordings of cardiovascular activity by placing a subject's hand in a water-filled
tank covered with a rubber membrane. Cardiovascular activity was reflected by
changes in the volume of the fist, which modulated the water level. Changes
in the water level were transferred by an air-filled tube to a revolving smoked
drum. Lombroso's measurement met with some success in detecting actual criminals.
The first "polygraph" for recording cardiac activity that contained the essential
features utilized in the modern polygraph for recording cardiovascular phenomena
was developed by heart specialist Sir James Mackenzie in 1906 (Mackenzie, 1908;
Gay, 1948). It was used, however, not for the psychophysiological detection
of concealed information but for medical purposes. As early as 1908 Harvard
professor Hugo Munsterberg (1908), an innovator in the psychophysiological study
of emotion, proposed the use of all three of the measurements now commonly used
in lie detection, along with several other behavioral and psychophysiological
parameters. His description of the techniques is both insightful and poetic.
If a girl blushes when a boy's name is mentioned in the family sitting-room,
we feel sure, even if she protests, that he is not quite indifferent to her
young heart. If she opens a letter and grows pale while reading it, she may
assure us that the event is unimportant; we know better . . .
Yes, the hidden feeling betrays itself often against the will . . . It may be
easy to suppress intentionally the conspicuous movements by which we usually
accentuate the emotions . . . But the lips and the hands and arms and legs,
which are under our control, are never the only witnesses to the drama which
goes on inside--if they keep silent, others will speak.
Our inspirations and expirations can be registered in finest detail and a variety
of elegant methods are available. Perhaps the simplest "pneumograph" consists
of a tube made of spiral wire and covered with rubber, to be attached by ribbons
to the chest . . . As soon as such delicate methods of registration are applied,
the intimate relation between feeling and breath becomes evident . . .
The same holds true for the heart beat, measured by the blood wave in the arteries;
such a pulse writer is called a sphygmograph. It may be attached, for instance,
to the wrist; a delicate lever presses against the wall of the blood vessel
just where the finger of the physician would feel the pulse . . . When we write
pulse and breathing together on the same drum, we see at once that even ordinary
inspiration changes the pulse; while we inhale we have a pulse different from
the pulse when we exhale. Far more influential are the feelings . . .
But there is still another way to observe the changes in our blood vessels.
We may examine the quantity of blood, for instance, which streams to a limb,
by means of the so-called plethysmograph . . . every emotional excitement speaks
in the blood supply of every limb.
But we may go still further and point to expressions of emotion which are entirely
beyond human senses. If we put our hands on two copper plates and make the weak
galvanic current of a battery run through the plates and our body, we can, with
the help of a delicate galvanometer, measure the slightest variations of the
resistance of the current. Experiment shows that such changes occur, indeed,
if our brain is excited; any emotional disturbance influences the resistance:
it seems that the activity of the sweat-glands in the skin is under the nervous
influence of our feelings, and the functioning of these glands alters the electrical
conditions. A word we hear may excite us and at once the needle of the galvanometer
becomes restless: there is no more uncanny betrayal of our inmost mind. (Munsterberg,
1908, p. 113)
A device incorporating all three of the measures included in a modern polygraph--pulse,
breathing, and skin conductance--was proposed to a congressional hearing by
Arthur McDonald in 1908, but such a device was not constructed until years later.
In 1914 Vittoria Benussi reported some success in detecting deception through
measuring changes in breathing patterns (Benussi, 1914). In 1915 William Marston
(Marston, 1917; 1938), a student of Munsterberg's and later also a professor
at Harvard, began a series of experiments using blood pressure in the detection
of deception. He used an ordinary sphygmomanometer to record blood pressure
periodically during questioning. Marston also recorded breathing patterns and
experimented with skin resistance measurements.
Burtt (1918) also experimented with respiratory measurements and blood pressure
in lie detection. He held the blood pressure changes to be of greater diagnostic
value.
The first continuous recording of blood pressure in interrogative polygraphy
was accomplished by Larson (1921; 1922). He developed an instrument that simultaneously
recorded blood pressure, pulse, and respiration continuously throughout a test
session. Larson's device was used extensively in criminal investigations, and
reportedly enjoyed a high degree of success (Larson; 1932).
In 1926 Keeler (1930) developed a machine that accomplished the same measurements
as Larson's and incorporated a number of technical improvements.
In 1938 Keeler incorporated the third major component of the modern polygraph,
namely the psychogalvanometer invented by Galvani in 1891. This provided for
the measurement of the skin's resistance to (or conductance of) an electrical
current, commonly known as electrodermal response (EDR), galvanic skin resistance
(GSR), or skin conductance response (SCR).
By this time, considerable research had been published on skin resistance, but
little of it had been related to the psychophysiological detection of concealed
information. An exception was Summers (1939), who conducted thousands of laboratory
experiments and investigated about 50 actual cases using galvanic skin resistance,
reportedly achieving a high level of accuracy in detecting deception or guilt.
Another early researcher in the use of galvanic skin resistance in psychophysiological
detection of concealed information was Wilson (Trovillo, 1939), who developed
a new recording psychogalvanometer in 1930 and used it in collaboration with
Keeler in several investigations. Shortly thereafter, the Chicago Police Crime
Detection Lab first used galvanic skin resistance in conjunction with blood
pressure measurements in the psychophysiological detection of concealed information.
It was on this background that Keeler developed the first instrument incorporating
the essential features of the modern polygraph. Keeler also refined the relevant-irrelevant
test, in which crime-relevant questions are interspersed with irrelevant questions.
He experimented with personally embarrassing questions and surprise questions
in an attempt to introduce a control stimulus that would evoke a reaction in
innocent as well as guilty subjects.
Summers (1939) provided the first descriptions of the control question technique,
in which a third type of question is employed that is designed to elicit an
emotional response in both guilty and innocent subjects for the sake of comparison
with the responses to relevant questions. Previously Larson (1921, p. 396) had
referred to a technique in which "a control question, or one not concerning
the subject under investigation, and yet calculated to stimulate various emotions,
was alternated with one pertinent to the investigation." He did not, however,
clearly distinguish between control and irrelevant questions.
Reid (1947; Reid & Inbau, 1977), often credited with first introducing the
control question technique, did in fact refine this technique to essentially
the same form in which it is practiced today. He also introduced the recording
of muscular activity as a means to detect attempted countermeasures. Reid and
his colleague, Fred Inbau (Reid & Inbau, 1977; Matte, 1980) did much to
systematize the methods for questioning and data acquisition in interrogative
polygraphy. Over the years they accumulated voluminous data on the applicability
and effectiveness of various techniques of conventional interrogative polygraphy.
INTERROGATIVE POLYGRAPHY: THE CURRENT
STATE OF THE ART
Types of Questions
The questions asked of the subject in %conventional interrogative polygraphy
fall into four categories: "relevant questions," "irrelevant questions," "control
questions," and "concealed information questions." Relevant questions are directly
related to the focus of an investigation. Irrelevant questions are irrelevant
to the investigation and are structured so as to have little or no emotional
significance for the subject. The response to these questions provides a baseline:
they establish the typical pattern with which the subject responds to routine
questions. Control questions are questions that, while not directly relevant
to the situation or issue under investigation, are designed to elicit an emotional
response from the subject that is similar to the response that the relevant
questions will elicit in a guilty subject. The control questions are concerned
with various undesirable behaviors in which the subject may have been involved
in the past, and are designed to elicit concern and/or doubt in the subject
about the veracity of his response. These questions are also used for the purpose
of comparison; they establish the typical physiological response of a subject
to questions about which he is concerned. Concealed information questions focus
on information about the crime or issue under investigation that would be known
only to the guilty party. It is assumed that a guilty individual will respond
more vigorously to (that is, be more physiologically aroused by) correct details
associated with the crime than to incorrect details, whereas innocent subjects
lacking in knowledge of the correct details will have the same response to both
classes of items.
The Three Classes of Conventional Interrogative
Polygraphy Tests
These four classes of questions are employed in three different interrogatory
systems: 1) the relevant/irrelevant test, 2) the control question test, and
3) the concealed information test.
1) The Relevant/Irrelevant Test
In the relevant/irrelevant test (Larson, 1922, 1932; Keeler, 1930), two types
of questions are presented: relevant and irrelevant. It is presumed that innocent
people will respond in a similar manner to the questions from both classes,
because they are unconcerned about the crime. Guilty persons will be more aroused
by the relevant questions, because they are more concerned about the crime.
This technique has serious drawbacks, the most obvious of which is that the
relevant questions concern subject matter that is inherently more upsetting
than the irrelevant questions. Some innocent people, as well as guilty individuals,
may respond to "Did you shoot John Jones last Tuesday?" or "Have you ever used
cocaine?" more strongly than to "Do you live on Cherry Street?" Therefore a
large response to the relevant questions may not be indicative of deception;
rather it may be the result of the emotional tendencies and the physiological
lability of the subject. Moreover, in the cases where the subject does not respond
to the relevant questions, there is no indication how the subjects would have
responded had the questions been about criminal activity of which the subject
was guilty. In short, there is no control, and as a result both false negatives
and false positives will occur. Because of this, the relevant/irrelevant technique
is seldom used in investigations of a specific crime or situation, although
it is still used in nonspecific investigations such as pre-employment and personnel
screening.
2) The Control Question Test
The most frequently employed technique in the investigation of specific crimes
or critical situations is the control question test, which was developed to
solve some of the problems of the relevant/irrelevant test (Reid & Inbau,
1977; Kircher, Horowitz, & Raskin, 1988). In the control question test,
control questions are added to relevant and irrelevant questions. These control
questions are designed to elicit an almost obligatory emotional response. It
is assumed that an innocent subject will respond more strongly to the control
questions, since he/she should not be concerned about the crime or issue at
hand. Guilty persons are expected to respond more to the relevant questions,
since they are more concerned with the specific issue under investigation than
with a general question about something undesirable they may have done in the
past.
Summers (1939) referred to the control questions as "emotional standards." Their
purpose was "to evoke within the individual rather intense psychogalvanic reactions
due to surprise, anger, shame, or anxiety over situations which he would ordinarily
prefer to conceal" (p. 341).
The usual control questions are somewhat vague, and cover a long period of time.
They are designed to encourage the subject to lie, or at least to be unsure
of the truthfulness of his or her answer (Reid & Inbau, 1977). Such control
questions are sometimes referred to as "probable lie" questions. An alternative
control question is the "directed lie" control question. These are questions
regarding past activities, usually very minor negative activities that everyone
is assumed to have committed, to which the subject is instructed to lie.
The control question test has been the subject of two classes of studies: 1)
field studies involving actual crimes (e.g., Raskin, 1976; Horvath & Reid,
1971; Barland & Raskin 1976; Bersh, 1969; Davidson, 1979; Horvath, 1977;
Hunter & Ash, 1973; Kleinmuntz & Szucko, 1984; Sicwick & Buckley,
1975; Wicklander & Hunter, 1975; Raskin, Barland, & Podlesny, 1978),
and 2) studies of mock crimes or similar deception-oriented circumstances conducted
in laboratories (e.g., Barland & Raskin 1975; Podlesny & Raskin 1978;
Raskin & Hare 1978; Rovner, Raskin, & Kircher, 1978; Widacki & Horvath
1978; Dawson 1980; Hammond, 1980; Bradley & Janisse 1981; Szucko & Kleinmuntz,
1981; Ginton, Dale, Elaad, & Ben-Shakhar, 1982; Kircher & Raskin, 1982;
Honts, Raskin, & Kircher, 1983; Kircher, 1983; Bradley & Ainsworth,
1984; Gatchel, Smith, & Kaplan, 1984; Honts, Hodes, & Raskin, 1985;
Forman & McCauley, 1986).
The results of these studies have been the subject of considerable controversy
over the effectiveness of the technique (see, for example, Lykken, 1978, 1979,
1988; Raskin, 1978, 1987, 1988; Raskin & Podlesny, 1979). Accuracy rates
for detecting mock guilty subjects have ranged from a low of 71% (Szucko &
Kleinmuntz, 1981) to a high of 100% (Dawson, 1980; Dale, Elaad, & Ben-Shakhar,
1982; Raskin & Hare, 1978). Accuracy for the detection of mock innocent
subjects has generally been found to be lower, ranging from 49% correct (Szucko
& Kleinmuntz, 1981) to 97% (Kircher & Raskin, 1982). A number of factors
may contribute to the differences in accuracy rates reported. Studies varied
widely in the type of subjects, the motivation of the subjects (higher motivation
typically yielded higher detection accuracy), access by examiners to information
(e.g., behavioral data) not included in the charts, the amount and type of psychophysiological
information provided to the interpreters, the skill of the interpreters, and
the method of scoring the charts.
One major difficulty with mock crime and other laboratory studies is that it
would be exceedingly difficult (not to mention highly unethical) to subject
individuals participating in a study to anything approaching the level of negative
emotional arousal experienced by individuals who are actually guilty of major
crimes in examinations attempting to detect that fact.
A few laboratory analog studies have provided a somewhat realistic experience.
Ginton, Dale, Elaad, and Ben-Shakhar (1982) gave cadets at the Israeli Police
Academy an opportunity to cheat on scoring a test they had taken, where the
paper was chemically treated to reveal any changes. The officers, some of whom
had cheated, were told that they were suspected of cheating, and offered an
opportunity to take a polygraph test. They were told that their careers might
depend on the outcome of the test. Such manipulations, though undoubtedly at
least somewhat effective in simulating an actual field polygraph examination,
are highly questionable on ethical grounds, and undoubtedly would not pass a
review by a human subjects committee in the United States.
Accuracy rates for field studies have varied even more than those of mock crime
studies. Accuracy of detection of criterion guilty individuals has ranged from
71% (Bersh, 1969) to 99% in one condition reported by Wicklander & Hunter
(1975). Innocent subjects have been detected with accuracies ranging from 12%
(with judicial outcome as the criterion of guilt, reported by Barland &
Raskin, 1976) to 94% in one condition reported by Bersh (1969). In addition
to many of the same variables affecting analog studies, field studies have varied
in the method of selection of cases, the type of crimes, the suspected role
in the crimes of the individuals examined, and the criterion for ground truth.
Ground truth is a particularly difficult issue, since in field studies it can
never be known with certainty. Confession is held by some to be the most accurate
criterion (Raskin, 1989), since it is rather unlikely that a suspect will confess
who is not indeed guilty. However, confessions are not independent of the polygraph
outcome, and tend to inflate the accuracy of both innocent and guilty determinations
by systematically excluding cases where an error has been made. If a guilty
subject is falsely found innocent, it is less likely that he or she will confess,
and more likely that the investigators will continue mistakenly to seek a suspect
who is guilty-which they will not find. Thus, no one will confess, the false
negative will go undetected, and the case will be excluded from the analysis.
If, on the other hand, an innocent suspect is falsely found guilty, and does
not confess, the investigators may tend to lessen their efforts to find the
guilty party since they think they already have found him. Again, no confession
takes place, the false positive goes undetected, and the case is excluded from
the analysis. (In the rare case in which an innocent subject does confess after
failing the polygraph test, he is mistakenly scored as a correct detection.)
The other common methods of establishing guilt or innocence in field studies
are judicial outcome and decision by a panel of experts. These methods, too,
have their share of difficulties (Barland, 1982; Raskin, 1988). The judicial
system in the United States, where virtually all of the field research has taken
place, is based on the principal of "innocent until proven guilty beyond a reasonable
doubt." Thus, both judges and juries and panels of experts who are trained in
this legal tradition often tend to err on the side of finding the subject innocent.
Also, in both judicial and panel decisions, the quantity of evidence available
for consideration may be low, in which case the subject also will generally
not be found guilty. This situation is a favorable one for human rights, of
course, but it renders these methods of determination of culpability problematical
for scientific purposes.
In an attempt to reduce some of the variability across examiners in the interpretation
of polygraph charts and to supplement examiner interpretation of polygraph data,
computerized statistical techniques have been developed which extract information
from polygraph data (Kircher, 1983; Kircher & Raskin, 1981, 1988). The accuracy
of this more objective approach is similar to the accuracy of examiner judgments.
The control question test is sometimes used in screening situations to investigate
particular classes of activities or future intentions. This involves questioning
similar to that employed in screening uses of the relevant/irrelevant technique,
with the addition of control questions. Such a use of the control question test
involves a difficult balance between control questions of a general nature designed
to elicit an emotional response in both innocent and guilty subjects, and relevant
questions, also often of a general nature, designed to elicit a response only
in the guilty.
A few studies have been conducted on the use of polygraphy in actual government
security screening applications. In a study by the Director of Central Intelligence
(U.S. CIA, DCI, 1982) on background investigations in the CIA, the polygraph
was used in an attempt to evaluate adverse information that had arisen regarding
individuals being considered for employment or security clearances. In two-thirds
of the cases where the information was resolved against the individual, the
individual admitted the adverse information. There is no indication of whether
the adverse information was correctly identified in the remaining one-third
of the cases; and since the polygraph was used only after a thorough investigation
had already revealed evidence against the individuals, it is impossible to know
precisely what role the polygraph played or how effective it would have been
in the absence of such an investigation and such pre-existing evidence. Edel
& Jacoby (1975) showed that polygraph examiner ratings of a "physiological
reaction" in applicants for U.S. government employment were reliable across
examiners; but they did not determine whether the reaction actually was indicative
of deception. In a laboratory study analogous to government security screening
applications that used military intelligence personnel as subjects, Barland
(1981) used the directed lie technique. Three different blind analysis techniques
were used to identify subjects who were lying to any question, and the same
three techniques were uses to identify which specific questions elicited lie
responses. The average accuracy in identifying both truthful/lying subjects
and truth/lie responses to specific questions was 69%.
3) Concealed Information Tests
Concealed information techniques are of two kinds: the guilty knowledge or concealed
knowledge test and the peak of tension test. Both work on the premise that a
guilty person will exhibit a larger response when confronted with correct details
relevant to the crime under investigation than to similar details that are unrelated
to the crime. An innocent person is expected to respond identically to both.
The guilty knowledge test has been proposed as an alternative to control question
techniques (Lykken, 1959, 1960, 1981, 1988; Giesen & Rollison, 1980; Ben-Shakhar,
Bar-Hillel, & Lieblich, 1986; Furedy & Helsgrave, 1988). Lykken (1959)
is often credited with the development of the guilty knowledge technique, although
in fact it was described as early as 1908 by Munsterberg (1908), and its successful
application was reported and analyzed in detail by Keeler (1930). In this technique,
subjects are asked a variety of questions about details of the crime that would
be known only to the guilty party. It is assumed that a guilty person will respond
differentially to the correct, crime-relevant details. Scientific studies of
the guilty knowledge test, like studies on the control question test, have produced
mixed results (Lykken, 1959; Davidson, 1968; Podlesny & Raskin, 1978; Balloun
& Holmes 1979; Giesen & Rollison, 1980; Bradley & Janisse, 1981;
Stern, Breen, Watanable, & Perry, 1981; Bradley and Warfield, 1984; Iacono,
Boisvenu, & Fleming, 1984; Elaad & Ben-Shakhar, 1989). It has been studied
almost exclusively in the laboratory. Because the innocent subject does not
know which are the relevant questions, the guilty knowledge test has often achieved
a very low level of false positives (e.g., 0% in Lykken, 1959; Davidson, 1968;
and Podlesny & Raskin, 1978). False negatives, however, have been a significant
problem, sometimes reaching levels of about 40% (e.g., Balloun & Holmes,
1979; Bradley & Janisse, 1981).
In the peak of tension test (Harrelson, 1964), a question concerning a relevant
and correct detail about a crime is embedded in a series of questions mentioning
similar but irrelevant details. The sequence of presentation of the questions
is known in advance to the subject. It is assumed that the physiological response
will peak at the time of the relevant question (Barland & Raskin, 1973;
Lykken, 1981).
Theory in Conventional Interrogative
Polygraphy
In the two thousand years since Erasistratos made his successful discoveries,
a large and rich data base has accumulated regarding the appearance and measurement
of the changes . . . indicative of the inward passions and inclinations of the
soul" (Plutarch, first century A.D./1952). Much more is now known about the
mechanism through which the autonomic nervous system, when activated by emotion,
brings about the changes measured by polygraphers. (See, for example, Lacey,
1967; Obrist, Webb, Sutterer, & Howard, 1970; Porges & Coles, 1976;
Schwartz, Weinberger, & Singer, 1981; Kandel and Schwartz, 1985; Coles,
Donchin, & Porges, 1986; Mathews, May, Mogg, & Eysenck, 1990; Davidson,
Ekman, Saron, Senulis, & Friesen, 1990; Ekman, Davidson, & Friesen,
1990; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990; Eysenck, Mogg, May, Richards, &
Mathews, 1991).
The fundamental psychophysiological theoretical foundation driving modern conventional
interrogative polygraphy, however, is fundamentally the same as that which led
Erasistratos to his early success in the psychophysiological detection of concealed
information. One addition of modern interrogative polygraphy is the understanding
of the adaptive value that these psychophysiological changes have in a physically
dangerous situation.
Emotions consist of intensified feelings regarding a situation, which are caused
by an interaction between the mind and the body resulting in physiological changes
within the body to cope with the situation . . . When these feelings are strong
. . . the accompanying physiological changes are extensive. Under strong fear,
these changes have the effect of preparing the body for a fight or flight .
. .
The system that prepares the body's defenses to meet these emergencies is...the
sympathetic subdivision of the autonomic nervous system . . . In a polygraph
situation, it is fear, fear of detection, fear of the consequences if the individual
is detected, that causes the sympathetic system to activate in order to prepare
the body to meet the emergency (Matte, 1980).
This modern description of the phenomenon is very similar to the ancient descriptions
advanced by Erasistratos, and also to the descriptions set forth by early interrogative
polygraphers. Munsterberg's (1908) explanations have been quoted above. Benussi
(1914) attributed the physiological changes he measured to "internal excitement."
Marston (1917, 1938) analyzed in some detail the effect of the emotions of fear
and rage on the sympathetic nervous system, and reached similar conclusions
(although some of the details of Marston's analysis have not found favor with
later experts). Similarly, Burtt (1921a, 1921b), after extensive experimentation
and debriefing of subjects, implicated "fear or excitement." Keeler (1930) held
that fear was the primary emotion behind the responses of a guilty subject.
There has been considerable controversy in recent years over the emotional/physiological
arousal paradigm in interrogative polygraphy. One major contributing factor
to this controversy may be the gulf that has developed between practicing polygraphers
and academic scientists.
The ancient proponents of the emotional/physiological arousal paradigm for psychophysiological
detection of concealed information were physician/scientists. Many of the early
proponents of the introduction of modern instrumentation in the service of this
paradigm in the first third of the twentieth century were scientists well within
the mainstream of current scientific theory and methodology. They used the same
equipment in interrogative polygraphy that they used for other, unrelated physiological
and psychophysiological research (Munsterberg, 1908). After the introduction
of the "polygraph" as a specific piece of machinery with the sole purpose of
lie detection, however, a new breed of experts arose. The original field of
expertise of these individuals was not physiology or psychophysiology but criminology
and law enforcement.
Their expertise was primarily developed not in the scientific laboratory but
in the applied, field setting, where both the theoretical and practical considerations
held to be important differed from those that caught the attention of the academic
psychophysiologists of the day. Critics and proponents alike agree that there
was little contact between practicing polygraphers and the academic community.
As Raskin (1979) stated, "It is interesting to note that field polygraphers
(Backster, 1962; Reid, 1947) have managed to develop effective procedures for
detection of deception in the field setting without the benefit of formal training
in psychology and physiology and with minimal contact with the academic--scientific
community."
Outside of the field of interrogative polygraphy, considerable attention was
given to distinguishing between the psychophysiological manifestations of different
emotions and developing a more detailed account of the psychophysiological signature
of different emotional responses (Ax, 1953; Vanderhoof & Clancy, 1962; Lacey,
1967; Lacey & Lacey, 1970; Obrist, Webb, Sutterer, & Howard, 1970; Porges
& Coles, 1976; Schwartz, Weinberger, & Singer, 1981; Mathews, May, Mogg,
& Eysenck, 1990; Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, & Friesen, 1990; Ekman,
Davidson, & Friesen, 1990; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990; Eysenck,
Mogg, May, Richards, & Mathews, 1991). It has been shown, for example, that
a unified theory of arousal is inadequate to explain some observed phenomena.
Psychophysiologists have distinguished between different responses such as the
orienting reflex and the defensive reflex: skin conductance increases in both
cases, whereas heart rate decreases with the orienting reflex and increases
with the defensive reflex.
Such distinctions, however, have by and large not been important for the actual
practice of conventional interrogative polygraphy (Reid & Inbau, 1977).
Established on the original theoretical foundation recognizing the link between
emotions and physiological responses, interrogative polygraphy has proceeded
primarily empirically (Department of Defense, 1984; Raskin, 1979).
A strong psychophysiological response of any one of a number of flavors has
been deemed sufficient to reveal that a relevant stimulus is emotionally arousing
for an individual. In practice, polygraphers have come to recognize a wide variety
of different, sometimes opposite responses as indicators of the state or states
of heightened emotion that are hypothesized to accompany deception (Reid &
Inbau, 1977).
A person's body functions at one physiological pace during chart time...whether
fast, slow, calm, "nervous," or excited. This pace will henceforth be referred
to as "norm." The instrument merely records this norm and any deviation therefrom.
A deviation from norm, commonly called a response or reaction, is the result
of a verbal stimulus (a question) . . . If a certain brain center interprets
the question to mean harm to a person's well-being, providing the question is
answered truthfully, a series of nerve impulses is generated. Specifically,
the brain has delegated responsibility for protection of the body to the autonomic
nervous system and its emergency subdivisions, the sympathetic and parasympathetic.
As this emergency system goes into action, a minor to major change is recorded
on a moving chart. (Ferguson & Miller, 1973, p. 146).
Although polygraphers have described in considerable detail the neurological
mechanisms through which emotions and the concomitant autonomic nervous system
activities are revealed in polygraph recordings (Ferguson & Miller, 1973),
and some have indicated the need for some degree of specificity in the emotions
elicited during a polygraph test (Matte, 1980), a fine-grained analysis of different
emotions and their concomitant psychophysiological responses has not been necessary
for the practice of conventional interrogative polygraphy. Some go so far as
to reject such analysis as irrelevant.
First and foremost, it must be emphatically stated that the polygraph examiner
is not concerned with attempting to differentiate one emotion from another.
He is only concerned that a verbal stimulus did provoke an emotion which produced
a sufficient nerve impulse to create a deviation from norm on the chart (Ferguson
& Miller, 1973, p. 168).
The primarily empirical approach of conventional interrogative polygraphy is
exemplified by Reid & Inbau (1977) in their authoritative text that has
become perhaps the greatest classic in the field. They describe in voluminous
detail the various psychophysiological responses that have proven in their extensive
experience and that of other experts to be indicative of deception. They present
a comprehensive and detailed account of the procedures that have been shown
to be most effective in producing these responses; considerable knowledge about
the art and science of interrogation aside from psychophysiological techniques;
numerous case histories; a thoughtful discussion of legal, philosophical and
moral issues; historical perspective; and reviews of research. The psychophysiological
theory regarding the connection between the mind, emotions, and body that underlies
psychophysiological detection of concealed information is only very briefly
mentioned.
Although polygraphy has in general been practiced with a minimum of theoretical
elaboration, there have been from time to time some notable exceptions to the
rule. A number of scientists, some of them expert practicing polygraphers as
well, have attempted to elaborate the emotional/physiological arousal paradigm
for lie detection. Some have attempted also to bridge the gap between contemporary
academic science and interrogative polygraphy as practiced.
In a classic paper on the subject, Davis (1961) outlined three specific theories
for the psychophysiological response measured by conventional interrogative
polygraphy. He called them the conditioned response theory, the conflict theory,
and the threat-of-punishment theory. According to the conditioned response theory,
"the critical questions play the role of conditioned stimuli, and evoke some
`emotional` response with which they have been associated in the past." One
difficulty with the conditioning theory is that it is insufficient to explain
psychophysiological responses to lying about rather trivial matters with which
the subject has had no experience in the past, such as which number is on a
card.
The conflict theory "would presume that a specially large physiologic disturbance
would occur when two incompatible reaction tendencies are aroused at the same
time." The assumption is that a subject has the habit of telling the truth,
but in a situation necessitating deception to maintain his/her innocence the
subject would simultaneously experience an opposite tendency.
The punishment or threat-of-punishment theory, which has more often been called
the fear theory, is the working understanding on which conventional polygraphy
is generally practiced (Reid & Inbau, 1977; Barland & Raskin, 1973).
The fear theory holds that "a person will give a large physiologic response
during lying because he anticipates serious consequences if he fails to deceive.
In common language it might be that he fails to deceive the machine operator
for the very reason that he fears he will fail. The `fear` would be the very
reaction detected." (Davis, 1961). To explain successful detection where the
consequences are rather trivial, for example in the card test described above,
one must interpret punishment rather broadly. Davis holds that perhaps the subject
is psychologically "punished" by the detection of even trivial information.
Barland & Raskin (1973) describe another theory that they call the arousal
theory. According to the arousal theory, "detection occurs because of the different
arousal value of the various stimuli."
These elaborations of the basic theoretical foundation for interrogative polygraphy
are not mutually exclusive. Moreover, it is likely that several different aspects
of the psychophysiological phenomenon underlying conventional lie detection
take place simultaneously (Davis, 1961; Barland & Raskin, 1973; Raskin,
1989).
Raskin (1979) hypothesizes that the response explained by the fear theory is
primarily a defensive reflex, and the response explained by the arousal theory
is primarily an orienting reflex. Which response is elicited depends on the
interrogative situation in situations involving a relatively high level of emotion--particularly
the response to relevant questions in a real-life application of the control
question technique--the fear theory and the accompanying defensive response
predominate. In situations involving a comparatively low level of emotion, the
arousal theory and the accompanying orienting response predominate. Such low-emotion
situations include mock interrogations, and especially mock interrogations using
the guilty knowledge test. (Note that virtually all of the published results
on the guilty knowledge test have been obtained in mock, laboratory situations.)
The response to control questions has also been explained in terms of the arousal
theory (Raskin, 1979).
The above hypothesis is in accord with research that has recorded somewhat different
physiological responses in different lie detection situations. The electrodermal
response is particularly effective in detecting the orienting reflex, and may
be unstable in situations of very high emotion (Reid & Inbau, 1966). Cardiovascular
and pulmonary responses, by contrast, are more marked in situations involving
very high emotional levels. This may explain why Reid & Inbau (1966) found
the cardiovascular and pulmonary measurements to be superior to the electrodermal
measurement for field use, but not in the laboratory. (Note, however, that in
the 1977 revision of their book, Reid and Inbau modified their conclusions to
include a somewhat more favorable evaluation of the electrodermal response.)
Other researchers (e.g., Podlesny & Raskin, 1977), on the basis of scientific
studies many of which were laboratory studies involving mock crimes, concluded
that the electrodermal response was superior. The predominance of the arousal
theory in low emotion situations, combined with the fact that the guilty knowledge
test has been studied almost exclusively in mock situations and is inherently
less stressful than the control question test, may similarly explain the finding
(Podlesny & Raskin, 1978; Balloun & Holmes, 1979; Bradley & Janisse,
1981; Iacono, Cerri, Patrick, & Fleming, 1987) that the electrodermal response
is the most, and perhaps the only, effective measurement in the guilty knowledge
test.
Different researchers have emphasized a different balance between the fear and
arousal responses. Marston (1917; 1932) attempted to exclude the arousal response
and focus on measurements that would emphasize the fear response. Ben-Shakar,
Lieblich, and Kugelmass (1975), in a low emotion laboratory study on the guilty
knowledge technique, attempted to minimize the fear response, and to obtain
discrimination of knowledge on the basis of the arousal theory. They formulated
the dichotomization hypothesis, which attempted to explain their results as
differential habituation of the electrodermal response to different categories
of stimuli (relevant and irrelevant).
There is no evidence in the scientific literature, however, that a guilty individual
accused of a serious crime will fail to display a fear response when confronted
during a guilty knowledge test with relevant details of the crime. On the contrary,
anecdotal field experience (Keeler, 1930, p. 49) indicates a "violent emotional
response." Nor is there convincing evidence that the arousal response can (or
should) be entirely excluded in an actual interrogation situation (see Raskin,
1979, 1987). Fortunately, the proponents of the various points of view within
the conventional paradigm agree that for practical purposes it does not matter.
As long as the subjects' responses are different in magnitude to the different
types of questions, the direction and specific details of the response are not
crucial for distinguishing guilt from innocence. As Lykken (1959), a major proponent
of the guilty knowledge test and severe critic of the control question test,
noted in describing the guilty knowledge test,
A guilty subject would be expected to respond differently to the relevant than
the irrelevant items. Usually, he would be expected to give larger responses
to the relevant items, although it should be pointed out that any consistent
difference in the responses to the two classes of stimuli is evidence of guilt.
Note that in this regard Lykken's view is very similar to that expressed by
Ferguson & Miller (1973), supporters of the control question test, as quoted
above in reference to that technique.
Conventional interrogative polygraphy, and in particular the control question
test, have been criticized on a number of scientific, ethical, humanitarian,
and legal grounds (Skolnick, 1961; OTA, 1983; Saxe, Dougherty, & Cross,
1985; Kleinmuntz & Szucko, 1982; Iacono, 1985;). There has been what might
be termed a lively interchange of ideas between proponents of the different
conventional polygraphy methods (see, for example, Lykken, 1978; Raskin, 1978;
Barland, 1985; Horvath, 1985). Many of the harshest critics of the control question
test are advocates of the guilty knowledge test (see, for example, Lykken, 1959,
1978, 1985, 1988; Furedy & Helsgrave, 1988; Ben-Shakhar, Bar-Hillel, &
Lieblich, 1986), and the reverse is also true (Raskin, 1989). Criticisms range
from a general statement that more research is needed to comparisons of control
question polygraphy with the superstitious bone-pointing procedures used by
Australian aborigines (Furedy, 1987). The evidence on the accuracy and validity
of the conventional polygraphy is mixed, with estimates of accuracy ranging
from chance to 100%. Both sides of the controversy (see, for example, Lykken,
1988; Raskin, 1988) conclude that the methodologically and theoretically sound
studies support their point of view, whereas the studies purporting to support
the opposite point of view are seriously flawed (Barland, 1985). There is general
agreement that the quality and experience of the examiner have a significant
effect on the outcome of a polygraph examination (Barland, 1988), although whether
this is a strength or a weakness of the technique is a matter of debate. There
also has been considerable controversy over who is qualified to speak with authority
on interrogative polygraphy--experienced field polygraphers or academic scientists.
The current state of the emotional/physiological arousal paradigm for lie detection,
then, is one of contradictions and controversy. Even the strongest proponents
of the current paradigm acknowledge that it has both theoretical and practical
weaknesses, while even its most vehement critics at least admit that conventional
polygraphy has some degree of effectiveness in eliciting confessions from guilty
subjects. In the next section I will compare the several applications of this
paradigm that are currently in use, discuss their strengths and weaknesses,
and contrast the current paradigm with the new paradigm reported herein.
Comparative Strengths and Weaknesses
of the Conventional Techniques
The Relevant/Irrelevant Test
The fundamental weakness of the relevant/irrelevant technique is that the dimension
of "relevant/irrelevant to this particular crime" is entirely confounded with
the dimension of "having to do with any crime or undesirable activity (and consequently
potentially upsetting)." This can result in either false positives or false
negatives (Podlesny & Raskin, 1977). If a subject is particularly responsive
to any mention of criminal or undesirable activity, then a false positive can
result. If an individual is particularly unresponsive to such mention, then
a false negative can result. Even many of the strongest supporters of conventional
polygraphy now agree that the relevant/irrelevant test is seriously flawed (Raskin,
1979).
The control question test attempts to deal with this confound by introducing
a class of questions, the control questions, which have to do with crimes or
undesirable activities on the part of the subject, but are not relevant to the
particular crime under investigation.
The Control Question Test
This aspect of control is the primary strength of the control question technique.
In introducing an additional type of question, however, the control question
test also inevitably introduces an additional source of variability. The control
questions are never fully comparable to the relevant questions. Ordinarily,
control questions are of a general nature and cover a long period of time. (For
example, "Have you ever stolen anything from anyplace where you worked?") Relevant
questions are often very specific. (For example, "Did you steal S200 from Smith's
wallet last Tuesday?") A false negative may result if an individual responds
excessively to questions of the type exemplified by the control questions. This
can be a result of the examiner's choice of control questions, the psychological
or physiological makeup of the subject, deliberate countermeasures, or any one
of a number of other factors. If, for any of the above reasons, an individual
tends to respond only slightly to the kind of questions of which the control
questions are examples, then a false positive may result. Similarly, exceptionally
large or small responses to specific crime-related questions can result in false
positives or negatives respectively. These difficulties can be ameliorated by
skillful interrogation and choice of control questions, but they are intrinsic
to the design of the control question test and can never be eliminated entirely.
It is generally agreed by critics and supporters alike (OTA, 1983; Barland,
1985) that the control question test is more effective at discovering guilty
subjects than at clearing innocent ones. This may be due, at least in part,
to the fact that the relevant questions are recognized as such by both innocent
and guilty subjects, and the relevant questions may tend to be inherently more
upsetting than the control questions regardless of the guilt or innocence of
the subject.
Another major difficulty of the control question test is that it can, and in
fact in order to be successful it must, subject an innocent subject to considerable
stress and negative emotional arousal. In the case of a guilty subject in an
actual criminal interrogation, field experience indicates that the relevant
questions are highly stressful in both the control question test (Gugas, 1979)
and the guilty knowledge test (Keeler, 1930). It may be argued, however, that
this stress is a result of the subject's criminal activity, rather than the
test per se, and that a similar level of stress would inevitably be incurred
in any interrogation of a guilty subject. For an innocent subject, however,
there are two additional sources of stress in the control question test. The
relevant questions, particularly if the crime is serious, may be experienced
as highly stressful. Moreover, the control questions are designed to elicit
a similar response in all subjects to the response to relevant questions in
a guilty subject. Since the responses to the relevant and control questions
are compared to make the determination of guilt or innocence, an innocent subject,
in order to pass the test, must exhibit larger responses to the control questions
than to the relevant questions. This means that the control questions must be
designed to produce an even larger response than questions that falsely accuse
a subject of a real crime.
Munsterberg (1908), one of the earliest modern advocates of the emotional/physiological
arousal paradigm for lie detection, summarized its limitations perhaps as eloquently
as anyone since.
. . experiment gives us so far not sufficient hold for the discrimination of
the guilty conscience and the emotional excitement of the innocent. The innocent
man, especially the nervous man, may grow as much excited on the witness stand
as the criminal when the victim and the means of the crime are mentioned; his
fear that he may be condemned unjustly may influence his muscles, glands and
blood vessels as strongly as if he were guilty. Experimental psychology cannot
wish to imitate with its subtle methods the injustice of barbarous police methods.
The real use of the emotion-method is therefore so far probably confined to
those cases in which it is to be found out whether a suspected person knows
anything about a certain place or man or thing.
The Guilty Knowledge Test
The greatest strength of the guilty knowledge test is that an innocent subject
cannot distinguish between the relevant and irrelevant items. This avoids a
major source of false positives that occur with the control question test: that
an innocent subject is emotionally aroused by the inherently upsetting and clearly
recognizable relevant questions. In a properly structured guilty knowledge test,
only a guilty subject can recognize the relevant questions as such, and consequently
an innocent subject is unlikely to respond differentially to the relevant items
(Lykken, 1988). The fact that the innocent subject does not know which are the
relevant questions, the absence of accusatory relevant questions, and the lack
of intentionally disturbing control questions also make the guilty knowledge
test less stressful for an innocent subject than the control question test.
The major weakness of the guilty knowledge test is its lack of control. If a
subject does not respond to the relevant questions, there may be at least two
different explanations: 1) the subject is innocent, and is not responding to
the relevant stimuli because they are no different to him than the irrelevant
stimuli, or 2) the subject is guilty, but for some physiological or psychological
reason does not show a differential response to questions relevant to his guilt.
The guilty knowledge test does not allow a distinction between these two possibilities.
If the subject does not respond, there is no way of determining what it would
have taken to make him respond. This feature makes the guilty knowledge test
particularly susceptible to false negatives (Raskin, 1988). (Note that the ERP-based
test reported here does not suffer from this weakness. The "target" stimuli
provide a control response that is lacking in the conventional guilty knowledge
test).
Another weakness of the guilty knowledge test is that it necessitates knowledge
of the details of the crime on the part of the examiner. In practice, this means
that considerably more time and effort must be invested by an interrogator in
the investigation. In some cases the guilty knowledge test can not be applied
at all. For example, the investigators may be unable to obtain sufficient information
to structure a test based on guilty knowledge; or a subject may admit being
present at the crime scene but maintain that he or she was a witness or that
no crime occurred..Another difficulty is that special care must be taken not
to provide the suspect with critical information during interrogation prior
to the test.
Because of these difficulties, the conventional guilty knowledge test has not
been widely implemented in actual investigations in the United States. In Japan
and Israel, however, it is much more widely employed.
Both the strengths and the weaknesses of the peak of tension technique are similar
to those of the guilty knowledge technique.
Of course, the skill of the examiner is an important factor in the effectiveness
of all conventional interrogative polygraphy techniques. Only a highly skilled
examiner can use any of these techniques effectively (Reid and Inbau, 1977;
Raskin, 1988).Each of the conventional techniques, however, suffers from particular
inherent difficulties. Moreover, all of them suffer from some common difficulties
derived from their dependence on the interactions between emotions and the autonomic
nervous system.
The Autonomic Nervous System in Interrogative
Polygraphy
All previous approaches to interrogative polygraphy are based on the assumption
that a deceptive action on the part of the subject will be accompanied by an
affective reaction. (For reviews, see Furedy, 1986; OTA, 1983; Department of
Defense, 1984; Kircher, Horowitz, & Raskin, 1988; Raskin, 1987, 1988, 1990;
Lykken, 1988). This reaction, in turn, is associated with activity in the autonomic
nervous system, an activity that is manifested by an ensemble of actions generated
within a variety of effectors driven by the autonomic nervous system. Some of
these actions can be detected by monitoring several biological systems whose
activity is modulated by the autonomic nervous system.
The use of affective responses in the detection of deception suffers from a
number of inherent difficulties. All conventional detection of deception techniques
depend on 1) the examiner's ability to create a situation that will effectively
elicit particular emotions in the subject--and in particular, different patterns
or levels of emotional arousal in innocent and guilty subjects--in the course
of an interrogation, 2) the subject's emotional responses, 3) the subject's
physiological responses when these emotions are aroused, 4) accurate measurement
of these physiological responses, and 5) the examiner's interpretation of these
physiological measurements.
Difficulties with Autonomic Nervous
System-Based Polygraphy
Even if it is assumed that autonomic nervous system responses can be accurately
measured and that the examiner will bring to the interpretation of these measurements
a high level of expertise, a number of difficulties inevitably arise in drawing
inferences about the guilt or innocence of a subject based on the above sequence
of events. A researcher or interrogator is never certain that even a carefully
structured and effectively executed interrogation will differentially elicit
the requisite level of emotional arousal at the appropriate times. Subjects
may exhibit unpredictable, unusual, or unexpected emotional responses (or lack
of responses), or may manipulate their emotions to avoid detection. Some types
of individuals (e.g., psychopaths) may not exhibit the emotions usually found.
Different individuals may exhibit similar emotions for different reasons. For
example, two individuals may be equally fearful in response to a particular
item, one because he committed a crime and is afraid of detection and another
because he did not commit the crime and is afraid of being falsely convicted.
Even if the emotional responses are as predicted and desired, autonomic nervous
system responses may not be. Subjects may control their physiological responses
voluntarily or through drugs, may employ hidden techniques (e.g., biting the
tongue) to bring about misleading responses, or may naturally have unusually
large or small autonomic .nervous system responses or idiosyncratic patterns
of electrodermal response, breathing, or cardiovascular activity. The difficulty
in inferring emotions from their autonomic nervous system "correlates" is well
known.
As has been reported in many studies of interrogative polygraphy, false positives
are often the consequence of an affective response to a question, or to the
presentation of an item, for reasons that have little or nothing to do with
deception (Lykken, 1974, 1978). Conversely, false negatives often result from
the absence, or the low amplitude, of an autonomic nervous system manifestation
of an affect.
It is also the case that only a portion of the variance in the autonomic nervous
system activity is driven by affective processes. The autonomic nervous system
is charged with the regulation of the most vital of the body's functions, and
changes in heart rate, blood pressure, respiration and sweat gland activity
reflect a variety of non-affective demands by the system. These include, for
example, varying demands for blood supply, variations in the need for oxygen,
and modulations of body temperature. From the point of view of interrogative
polygraphy the autonomic nervous system is quite a noisy system. The "signal,"
that is, the autonomic nervous system activity driven by affect, may be swamped
or masked by the "noise," the autonomic nervous system activity that is related
to the vegetative and energetic functions of the system.
These factors are, in part, responsible for the ability of individuals to modify
autonomic nervous system at will. The susceptibility of the autonomic nervous
system for control using biofeedback makes it even more vulnerable, especially
for specially trained individuals, to voluntary control by the subject. Countermeasures
may range from the voluntary generation of affect by cognitive control to the
activation of bodily parts so as to create energy needs that will be reflected
in autonomic nervous system responses at critical points in the polygraph examination.
Comments