Patent No. 6553252 Method and system for predicting human cognitive performance
Patent No. 6553252
Method and system for predicting human cognitive performance (Balkin, et al., Apr 22, 2003)
Abstract
An apparatus and method for predicting cognitive performance of an individual based on factors including sleep history and the time of day. The method facilitates the creation of predicted cognitive performance curves that allow an individual to set his/her sleep times to produce higher levels of cognitive performance. The method also facilitates the reconstruction of past cognitive performance levels based on sleep history.
Notes:
FIELD
OF THE INVENTION
This invention relates to a method for predicting cognitive performance of an
individual based on that individual's prior sleep/wake history and the time
of day.
BACKGROUND OF THE INVENTION
Maintenance of productivity in any workplace setting depends upon effective
cognitive performance at all levels from command/control or management down
to the individual soldier or worker. Effective cognitive performance in turn
depends upon complex mental operations. Many factors have been shown to affect
cognitive performance (e.g., drugs or age). However, of the numerous factors
causing day to day variations in cognitive performance, two have been shown
to have the greatest impact. These two factors are an individual's prior sleep/wake
history and the time of day.
Adequate sleep sustains cognitive performance. With less than adequate sleep,
cognitive performance degrades over time. An article by Thorne et al. entitled
"Plumbing Human Performance Limits During 72 hours of High Task Load" in Proceedings
of the 24.sup.th DRG Seminar on the Human as a Limiting Element in Military
Systems, Defense and Civil Institute of Environmental Medicine, pp. 17-40 (1983),
an article by Newhouse et al. entitled "The Effects of d-Amphetamine on Arousal,
Cognition, and Mood After Prolonged Total Sleep Deprivation" published in Neuropsychopharmacology,
vol. 2, pp. 153-164 (1989), and another article by Newhouse et al. entitled
"Stimulant Drug Effects on Performance and Behavior After Prolonged Sleep Deprivation:
A Comparison of Amphetamine, Nicotine, and Deprenyl" published in Military Psychology,
vol. 4, pp. 207-233 (1992) all describe studies of normal volunteers in which
it is revealed that robust, cumulative decrements in cognitive performance occur
during continuous total sleep deprivation as measured by computer-based testing
and complex operational simulation. In the Dinges et al. article entitled "Cumulative
Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements
During a Week of Sleep Restricted to 4-5 Hours Per Night" published in Sleep,
vol. 20, pp. 267-277 (1997) it is revealed that on fixed, restricted daily sleep
amounts, cumulative reduced sleep also leads to a cognitive performance decline.
Thus, in operational settings, both civilian and military, sleep deprivation
reduces productivity (output of useful work per unit of time) on cognitive tasks.
Thus, using computer-based cognitive performance tests, it has been shown that
total sleep deprivation degrades human cognitive performance by approximately
25% for each successive period of 24 hours awake. However, it also has been
shown that even small amounts of sleep reduce the rate of sleep loss-induced
cognitive performance degradation: Belenky et al. in their article entitled
"Sustaining Performance During Continuous Operations: The U.S. Army's Sleep
Management System," published in 20.sup.th Army Science Conference Proceedings,
vol. 2, pp. 657-661 (1996), disclose that a single 30-minute nap every 24 hours
reduces the rate of cognitive performance degradation to 17% per day over 85
hours of sleep deprivation. This suggests that recuperation of cognitive performance
during sleep accrues most rapidly early in the sleep period. No other factor
besides the amount of sleep contributes so substantially and consistently to
the normal, daily variations in cognitive performance.
In addition to sleep/wake history, an individual's cognitive performance at
a given point in time is determined by the time of day. In the early 1950s,
Franz Halberg and associates observed a 24-hour periodicity in a host of human
physiologic (including body temperature and activity), hematologic, and hormonal
functions, and coined the term `circadian` (Latin for `about a day`) to describe
this cyclic rhythm. Halberg showed that most noise in experimental data came
from comparisons of data sampled at different times of day.
When humans follow a nocturnal sleep/diurnal wake schedule (for example, an
8-hour sleep/16-hour wake cycle, with nightly sleep commencing at approximately
midnight), body temperature reaches a minimum (trough) usually between 2:00
AM and 6:00 AM. Body temperature then begins rising to a maximum (peak) usually
between 8:00 PM and 10:00 PM. Likewise, systematic studies of daily human cognitive
performance rhythms show that speed of responding slowly improves across the
day to reach a maximum in the evening (usually between 8:00 PM and 10:00 PM)
then dropping more rapidly to a minimum occurring in the early morning hours
(usually between 2:00 AM and 6:00 AM). Similar but somewhat less consistent
rhythms have been shown from testing based on various cognitive performance
tasks. Thus, superimposed on the effect of total sleep deprivation on cognitive
performance noted above was an approximately .+-.10% percent variation in cognitive
performance over each 24-hour period.
Various measures have been shown to correlate, to some extent, with cognitive
performance. These include objective and subjective measures of sleepiness (or
its converse, alertness). Some individuals familiar with the art use "sleepiness"
to indicate the opposite of "alertness" (as is the case in the present document).
"Drowsiness" often is used interchangeably with "sleepiness" although some familiar
with the art would argue that "sleepiness" pertains specifically to the physiological
need for sleep whereas "drowsiness" refers more to the propensity or ability
to fall asleep (independent of physiological sleep need) or the subjective feeling
of lack of alertness. The term "fatigue" has been used as a synonym for "sleepiness"
by the lay population, but those familiar with the art do not consider "fatigue"
to be interchangeable with "sleepiness"--rather, "fatigue" is a broad term that
encompasses more than just the effects of sleep loss per se on performance.
Likewise, "cognitive performance" has been defined as performance on a wide
variety of tasks, the most commonly used being vigilance tasks (tasks requiring
sustained attention). From vigilance and other tasks, some researchers use accuracy
as their measure of cognitive performance, while others use reaction time (or
its inverse, speed). Still others use a measure that is calculated as speed
multiplied by accuracy, that is the amount of useful work performed per unit
of time (also known as throughput). Those familiar with the art generally agree
that vigilance tasks are appropriate measures of cognitive performance under
conditions of sleep deprivation, and that either reaction time (speed) or some
measure that takes reaction time into account (e.g., throughput) is a valid
and reliable way of measuring cognitive performance.
The Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) is a widely accepted objective measure
of sleepiness/alertness. In the MSLT, individuals try to fall asleep while lying
in a darkened, quiet bedroom. Various physiological measures used to determine
sleep or wakefulness are recorded (eye movements, brain activity, muscle tone),
and time taken to reach the first 30 seconds of stage 1 (light) sleep is determined.
Shorter latencies to stage 1 are considered to indicate greater sleepiness (lower
alertness). Sleep latencies under 5 minutes are considered to be pathological
(i.e., indicative of a sleep disorder or sleep deprivation). During both total
and partial sleep deprivation, latency to sleep on the MSLT (alertness) and
performance decline (i.e., sleepiness as measured by MSLT increases). However,
although there is a correlation between MSLT-determined sleepiness/alertness
and cognitive performance (greater sleepiness as indexed by MSLT corresponding
to poorer cognitive performance), this correlation has never been shown to be
perfect and for the most part is not strong. As a result, the MSLT is a poor
(i.e., unreliable) predictor of cognitive performance.
Subjective measures of sleepiness/alertness also have been shown to correlate
(albeit weakly) with cognitive performance. Hoddes et al., in their article
entitled "Quantification of Sleepiness: A New Approach" published in Psychophysiology,
vol. 10, pp. 431-436 (1973) describe the Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS), a
subjective questionnaire used widely to measure sleepiness/alertness. In the
SSS, individuals rate their current level of sleepiness/alertness on a scale
from 1 to 7, with 1 corresponding to the statement, "feeling active and vital;
alert; wide awake" and 7 corresponding to the statement "almost in reverie;
sleep onset soon; losing struggle to remain awake." Higher SSS scores indicate
greater sleepiness. As with the MSLT, during both total and partial sleep deprivation,
scores on the SSS increase. However, as with MSLT, the correspondence between
SSS-determined sleepiness/alertness and cognitive performance decrements is
weak and inconsistent. As a result, the SSS also is a poor predictor of cognitive
performance. Some other examples of subjective measures of sleepiness/alertness
include the Epworth Sleepiness Scale described by Johns in his article entitled
"Daytime Sleepiness, Snoring, and Obstructive Sleep Apnea" published in Chest,
vol. 103, pp. 30-36 (1993), and the Karolinska Sleepiness scale described by
Akerstedt and Gillberg in their article entitled "Subjective and Objective Sleepiness
in the Active Individual" published in International Journal of Neuroscience,
vol. 52, pp. 29-37 (1990). The correspondence between these subjective measures
and cognitive performance also is weak and inconsistent.
In addition, factors modifying cognitive performance may not correspondingly
affect objective or subjective measures of sleepiness/alertness, and vice versa.
For example, the Penetar et al. article entitled "Amphetamine Effects on Recovery
Sleep Following Total Sleep Deprivation" published in Human Psychopharmacology,
vol. 6, pp. 319-323 (1991), disclose that during sleep deprivation, the stimulant
drug d-amphetamine improved cognitive performance but not sleepiness/alertness
(as measured by the MSLT). In a similar study, caffeine given as a sleep deprivation
countermeasure maintained elevated cognitive performance for over 12 hours while
the effects on subjective sleepiness, vigor and fatigue transiently improved
but then decayed. Thorne et al. in their article entitled "Plumbing Human Performance
Limits During 72 hours of High Task Load" in Proceedings of the 24.sup.th DRG
Seminar on the Human as a Limiting Element in Military Systems, Defense and
Civil Institute of Environmental Medicine, pp. 17-40 (1983), describe how cognitive
performance continues to decline over 72 hours of sleep deprivation whereas
subjective sleepiness/alertness declined over the first 24 hours but subsequently
leveled off. The findings that cognitive performance and measures of sleepiness/alertness
are not always affected in the same way indicate that they are not interchangeable.
That is, measures of sleepiness/alertness cannot be used to predict cognitive
performance, and vice versa.
Methods and apparatuses related to alertness detection fall into five basic
categories: a method/apparatus for unobtrusively monitoring current alertness
level; a method/apparatus for unobtrusively monitoring current alertness level
and providing a warning/alarm to the user of decreased alertness and/or to increase
user's alertness level; a method/apparatus for monitoring current alertness
level based on the user's responses to some secondary task possibly with an
alarm device to warn the user of decreased alertness and/or to increase user's
alertness level; methods to increase alertness; and a method/apparatus for predicting
past, current, or future alertness.
These methods and apparatuses that unobtrusively monitor the current alertness
level are based on an "embedded measures" approach. That is, such methods infer
alertness/drowsiness from the current level of some factor (e.g., eye position
or closure) assumed to correlate with alertness/drowsiness. Some recently issued
patents of this type include U.S. Pat. No. 5,689,241 to J. Clarke, Sr., et al.
disclosing an apparatus to detect eye closure and ambient temperature around
the nose and mouth; U.S. Pat. No. 5,682,144 to K. Mannik disclosing an apparatus
to detect eye closure; and U.S. Pat. No. 5,570,698 to C. Liang et al. disclosing
an apparatus to monitor eye localization and motion to detect sleepiness. An
obvious disadvantage of these types of methods and apparatuses is that the measures
are likely detecting sleep onset itself rather than small decreases in alertness.
In some patents, methods for embedded monitoring of alertness/drowsiness are
combined with additional methods for signaling the user of decreased alertness
and/or increasing alertness. Recently issued patents of this type include U.S.
Pat. No. 5,691,693 to P. Kithil describing a device that senses a vehicle operator's
head position and motion to compare current data to profiles of "normal" head
motion and "impaired" head motion. Warning devices are activated when head motion
deviates from the "normal" in some predetermined way. U.S. Pat. No. 5,585,785
to R. Gwin et al. describes an apparatus and a method for measuring total handgrip
pressure on a steering wheel such that an alarm is sounded when the grip pressure
falls below a predetermined "lower limit" indicating drowsiness. U.S. Pat. No.
5,568,127 to H. Bang describes a device for detecting drowsiness as indicated
by the user's chin contacting an alarm device, which then produces a tactile
and auditory warning. U.S. Pat. No. 5,566,067 to J. Hobson et al. describes
a method and an apparatus to detect eyelid movements. A change in detected eyelid
movements from a predetermined threshold causes an output signal/alarm (preferably
auditory). As with the first category of methods and apparatuses, a disadvantage
here is that the measures are likely detecting sleep onset itself rather than
small decreases in alertness.
Other alertness/drowsiness monitoring devices have been developed based on a
"primary/secondary task" approach. For example, U.S. Pat. No. 5,595,488 to E.
Gozlan et al. describes an apparatus and a method for presenting auditory, visual,
or tactile stimuli to an individual to which the individual must respond (secondary
task) while performing the primary task of interest (e.g., driving). Responses
on the secondary task are compared to baseline "alert" levels for responding.
U.S. Pat. No. 5,259,390 to A. MacLean describes a device in which the user responds
to a relatively innocuous vibrating stimulus. The speed to respond to the stimulus
is used as a measure of the alertness level. A disadvantage here is that the
apparatus requires responses to a secondary task to infer alertness, thereby
altering and possibly interfering with the primary task.
Other methods exist solely for increasing alertness, depending upon the user
to self-evaluate alertness level and activate the device when the user feels
drowsy. An example of the latter is U.S. Pat. No. 5,647,633 and related patents
to M. Fukuoka in which a method/apparatus is described for causing the user's
seat to vibrate when the user detects drowsiness. Obvious disadvantages of such
devices are that the user must be able to accurately self-assess his/her current
level of alertness, and that the user must be able to correctly act upon this
assessment.
Methods also exist to predict alertness level based on user inputs known empirically
to modify alertness. U.S. Pat. No. 5,433,223 to M. Moore-Ede et al. describes
a method for predicting the likely alertness level of an individual at a specific
point in time (past, current or future) based upon a mathematical computation
of a variety of factors (referred to as "real-world" factors) that bear some
relationship to alterations in alertness. The individual's Baseline Alertness
Curve (BAC) is first determined based on five inputs and represents the optimal
alertness curve displayed in a stable environment. Next, the BAC is modified
by alertness modifying stimuli to arrive at a Modified Baseline Alertness Curve.
Thus, the method is a means for predicting an individual's alertness level,
not cognitive performance.
Another method has been designed to predict "work-related fatigue" as a function
of number of hours on duty. Fletcher and Dawson describe their method in an
article entitled "A Predictive Model of Work-Related Fatigue Based on Hours
of Work" published in Journal of Occupational Health and Safety, vol. 13, 471-485
(1997). In this model a simplifying assumption is made--it is assumed that length
of on-duty time correlates positively with time awake. To implement the method,
the user inputs a real or hypothetical on-duty/off-duty (work/rest) schedule.
Output from the model is a score that indicates "work-related fatigue." Although
this "work-related fatigue" score has been shown to correlate with some performance
measures, it is not a direct measure of cognitive performance per se. It can
be appreciated that the fatigue score will be less accurate under circumstances
when the presumed relationship between on-duty time and time awake breaks down--for
example when a person works a short shift but then spends time working on projects
at home rather than sleeping or when a person works long shifts but conscientiously
sleeps all the available time at home. Also, this method is obtrusive in that
the user must input on-duty/off-duty information rather than such information
being automatically extracted from an unobtrusive recording device. In addition,
the model is limited to predictions of "fatigue" based on work hours. Overall,
this model is limited to work-related situations in which shift length consistently
correlates (inversely) with sleep length.
Given the importance of the amount of sleep and the time of day for determining
cognitive performance (and hence estimating productivity or effectiveness),
and given the ever-increasing requirements of most occupations on cognitive
performance, it is desirable to design a reliable and accurate method of predicting
cognitive performance. It can be appreciated that increasing the number of relevant
inputs increases cognitive performance prediction accuracy. However, the relative
benefits gained from such inputs must be weighed against the additional burdens/costs
associated with their collection and input. For example, although certain fragrances
have been shown to have alertness-enhancing properties, these effects are inconsistent
and negligible compared to the robust effects of the individual's sleep/wake
history and the time of day. More important, the effect of fragrances on cognitive
performance is unknown. Requiring an individual to keep a log of exposure to
fragrances would be time consuming to the individual and only result in negligible
gains in cognitive performance prediction accuracy. In addition, while the effects
of the sleep/wake history and the time of day on cognitive performance are well
known, the effects of other putative alertness-altering factors (e.g., job stress
and workload), how to measure them (their operational definition), and their
direction of action (cognitive performance enhancing or degrading) are virtually
unknown.
An important and critical distinction between the present invention and the
prior art is that the present invention is a model to predict performance on
tasks with a cognitive component. In contrast, previous models involving sleep
and/or circadian rhythms (approximately 24-hour) focused on the prediction of
"alertness" or "sleepiness." The latter are concepts that specifically relate
to the propensity to initiate sleep, not the ability to perform a cognitive
task.
Although sleepiness (or its converse, alertness) could be viewed as an intervening
variable that can mediate cognitive performance, the scientific literature clearly
shows that cognitive performance and alertness are conceptually distinct, as
reviewed by Johns in the article entitled, "Rethinking the Assessment of Sleepiness"
published in Sleep Medicine Reviews, vol. 2, pp. 3-15 (1998), and as reviewed
by Mitler et al. in the article entitled, "Methods of Testing for Sleepiness"
published in Behavioral Medicine, vol. 21, pp. 171-183 (1996). Thomas et al.
in the article entitled "Regional Cerebral Metabolic Effects of Prolonged Sleep
Deprivation" published in Neurolmage, vol. 7, p. S130 (1998) reveal that 1-3
days of sleep loss result in reductions in global brain activation of approximately
6%, as measured by regional cerebral glucose uptake. However, those regions
(heteromodal association cortices) that mediate the highest order cognitive
functions (including but not limited to attention, vigilance, situational awareness,
planning, judgment, and decision making) are selectively deactivated by sleep
loss to a much greater extent--up to 50%--after three days of sleep loss. Thus,
decreases in neurobiological functioning during sleep restriction/deprivation
are directly reflected in cognitive performance degradation. These findings
are consistent with studies demonstrating that tasks requiring higher-order
cognitive functions, especially those tasks requiring attention, planning, etc.
(abilities mediated by heteromodal association areas) are especially sensitive
to sleep loss. On the other hand, brain regions such as primary sensory regions,
are deactivated to a lesser degree. Concomitantly, performance (e.g., vision,
hearing, strength and endurance tasks) that is dependent on these regions is
virtually unaffected by sleep loss.
Consequently, devices or inventions that predict "alertness" per se (e.g., Moore-Ede
et al.) putatively quantify the brain's underlying propensity to initiate sleep
at any given point in time. That is, devices or inventions that predict "alertness"
(or its converse "sleepiness") predict the extent to which sleep onset is likely.
The present invention differs from such approaches in that the nature of the
task is accounted for--i.e., it is not the propensity to initiate sleep that
is predicted. Rather, the present invention predicts the extent to which performance
of a particular task will be impaired by virtue of its reliance upon brain areas
most affected by sleep deprivation (heteromodal association areas of the brain).
The most desirable method will produce a highly reliable and accurate cognitive
performance estimate based on the sleep/wake history of an individual and the
time of day.
Comments